¿Y si el lenguaje tiene cien mil años? Explorando las consecuencias de la datación del FOXP2 humano
Resumen
And if language is 100,000 years old? Exploring the consequences of the paleogenitic analysis of human FOXP2
A recent paleogenetic analysis of FOXP2 relates the mutations of the human version of this gene to the emergence of modern humans, some 100-200 thousand years ago. This articles explores the consequences of such discovery concerning our understanding of the evolutionary process that brought language into existence. It is organized around three main questions: did Neanderthals use a type of language similar to ours?, what is the meaning of the morphologic and arqueologic data traditionally used to argue for or against the use of language among hominids?, and how can we explain the apparent mismatch between the emergence of language and the symbolic explosion of the Upper Paleolithic?
Key words: Origin of language, paleogenetic, paleoanthropology, modern sapiens, Neanderthal man, hominids, symbolic explosion.
Texto completo:
PDFReferencias
Alexander, M.P; Naeser, M.A; & Palumbo, C.L. (1987), “Correlations of subcortical CT lesion sites and aphasia profiles,” Brain 110: 961-991.
Arensburg, B.; Schepartz, L.A.; Tillier, A.M.; Vandermeersch, B.; & Rak, Y. (1990), “A reappraisal of the anatomical basis for speech in middle Palaeolithic hominids,” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 83(2): 137-146.
Arsuaga, Juan Luis (1999), El collar del neandertal. En busca de los primeros pensadores. Barcelona: Nuevas Ediciones de Bolsillo, 2000.
Arsuaga, Juan Luis; & Martínez, Ignacio (1998), La especie elegida. La larga marcha de la evolución humana. Madrid: Temas de Hoy.
Ayala, Francisco J.; & Cela Conde, Camilo José (2006), La piedra que se volvió palabra. Las claves evolutivas de la humanidad. Madrid: Alianza.
Benítez Burraco, Antonio (2005a), “FOXP2: del trastorno específico a la biología molecular del lenguaje. I. Aspectos etiológicos, neuroanatómicos, neurofisiológicos y moleculares”, Revista de Neurología 40 (11): 671-682.
Benítez Burraco, Antonio (2005a), “FOXP2: del trastorno específico a la biología molecular del lenguaje. II. Implicaciones para la ontogenia y filogenia del lenguaje”, Revista de Neurología 41 (1): 37-44.
Berwick, Robert C. (1998), “Language evolution and the Minimalist Program: The origins of syntax,” in J.R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy, and Ch. Knight (ed.) Approaches to the Evolution of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 320-340.
Bickerton, Derek (1990), Lenguaje y especies. Madrid: Alianza, 1994.
Bickerton, Derek (1995), Language and Human Behavior. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Botha, Rudolf Philip (2003), Unravelling the Evolution of Language. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Carstairs-McCarthy, Andrew (1999), The Origins of Complex Language. An Inquiry into the Evolutionary Beginnings of Sentences, Syllabes, and Truth. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cela Conde, Camilo José; & Ayala, Francisco (2001), Senderos de la evolución humana. Madrid: Alianza.
Chomsky, Noam (2000a), La arquitectura del lenguaje. Barcelona: Kairós, 2003.
Chomsky, Noam (2000b), “Minimalist inquiries: The framework,” in R. Martin, R., D. Michaels, D. and J. Uriagereka (eds.), Step by Step. Essays on Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik. Cambridge (MA): The MIT Press, pp. 89-155.
Coppens, Yves (2000), La rodilla de Lucy. Los primeros pasos hacia la humanidad. Barcelona: Tusquets, 2005.
Coqueugniot, H.; Hublin, J.-J.; Veillon, F.; Houët, F.; & Jacob, T. (2004), “Early brain growth in Homo erectus and implications for cognitive ability,” Nature 431: 299-302.
Corballis, Michael C. (2002), From Hand to Mouth. The Origins of Language. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Cosmides, Leda; & Tooby, John (2000), “Consider the source: The evolution of adaptations for decoupling and metarepresentation,” in Dan Sperber (ed.), Metarepresentations. A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 53-115.
Deacon, Terrence (1997), The Symbolic Species. The Co-Evolution of Language and the Human Brain. London: Penguin.
DeGusta, D.; Gilbert, W.H.; & Turner, S.P. (1999), “Hypoglossal canal size and hominid speech,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 96: 18001804.
Dorus, Steve; Vallender, Eric J.; Evans, Patrick D.; Anderson, Jeffrey R.; Gilbert, Sandra L.; Mahowald, Michael; Wyckoff, Gerald J.; Malcom, Christine M.; & Lahn, Bruce T. (2004), “Accelerated evolution of nervous system genes in the origin of Homo sapiens,” Cell 119: 1027-1040.
Eccles, John C. (1989), La evolución del cerebro: Creación de la conciencia. Barcelona: Labor, 1992.
Enard, W.; Przeworki, M.; Fischer, S.E; Lai, C.S.; Wiebe, V.; Kitano, T.; Monaco, A.P.; & Pääbo, S. (2002), “Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language,” Nature 418: 868-872.
Evans, Patrick D.; Gilbert, Sandra L.; Mekel-Bobrov, Nitzan; Vallender, Eric J.; Anderson, Jeffrey R.; Vaez-Azizi, Leila M.; Tishkoff, Sarah A.; Hudson, Richard R.; & Lahn, Bruce T. (2005), “Microcephalin, a gene regulatin brain size, continues to evolve adaptively in humans,” Science 309: 1717-1720.
Falk, Dean (1990), “The radiator hipótesis,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 333-381.
Ferland, R.J.; Cherry, T.J.; Preware, P.O.; Morrisey, E.E.; & Walhs, Ch.A (2003), “Characterization of FOXP2 and Foxp1 ARNm and protein in the developing and mature brain,” Journal of Comparative Neurology 460(2): 266-279.
Frayer, David W. & Nicolay, Chris (2000), “Fossil evidence for the origin of speech sounds,” in Nils L. Wallin, Björn Merker & Steven Brown (eds.), The Origins of Music. Cambridge (MA): The MIT Press, pp. 217-232.
Frutos, Rosa de; Sanjuán, Julio; & Tolosa, Amparo (2005), “Origen y genética del lenguaje,” en Julio Sanjuán & Camilo José Cela Conde (eds.), La profecía de Darwin. Del origen de la mente a la psicopatología. Barcelona: Ars Médica, pp. 83-103.
Haesler, Sebastian; Wada, Kazuhiro; Nshdejan, A.; Morrisey, Edward; Lints, Thierry; Jarvis, Eric D.; & Scharff, Constance (2004), “FOXP2 expression in avian vocal learners and non-learners,” The Journal of Neuroscience 24(13): 3164-3175.
Hockett, Charles F. (1960), “The origin of speech,” Scientific American 203/3: 88-96.
Hofreiter, M.; Serre, D.; Poinar, H.N.; Kuch, M.; & Pääbo (2001), “Ancient DNA,” Nature Reviews 2: 353-359.
Holloway, R. L. (1983), “Human paleontological evidence relevant to language behavior,” Human Neurobiology 2: 105-114.
Jungers, W.L.; Pokempker, Amy A.; Kay, R.; & Cartmill, M. (2003), “Hypoglossal canal size in living hominoids and the evolution of human speech,” Human Biology 75(4): 473-484.
Kappelman, J. (1996), “the evolution of body mass and relative brain size in fossil hominids,” Journal of Human Evolution 30: 243-276.
Kay, R.F.; Cartmill, M.; & Balow, M. (1998), “The hypoglossal canal and the origin of human vocal behavior,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 95: 5417-5419.
Krings, M.; Geisert, H.; Schmitz, R.W.; Krainitzki, H.; & Pääbo, S. (1999), “DNA Sequence of the Mitochondrial Hypervariable Region II from the Neandertal Type Specimen,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 96: 55815585.
Krings, M.; Stone, A.; Schmitz, R.W.; Krainitzki, H.; Stoneking, M.; & Pääbo, S. (1997), “Neandertal DNA sequences and the origin of modern humans,” Cell 90: 19-30.
Lai, Cecilia S.; Fisher, Simon E.; Hurst, Jane A.; Vargha-Khandem, Faraneh; & Monaco, Anthony, P. (2001), “A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder,” Nature 413: 519-523.
Lai, Cecilia S.; Gerrelli, D.; Monaco A.P.; Fisher, S.E.; & Copp, A.J. (2003), “FOXP2 expression during brain development coincides with adult sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disorder,” Brain 126: 2455-2462.
Laitman, J. (1984), “The anatomy of human speech,” Natural History 92: 20-27.
Lalueza Fox, Carlos (2005), Genes de neandertal. Madrid: Síntesis.
Leakey, Richard E. (1981), La formación de la humanidad. Barcelona: Ediciones del Aguazul, 2005.
Lieberman, Philip (1984), The Biology and Evolution of Language. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.
Lieberman, Philip (1998), Eve Spoke. Human Language and Human Evolution. New York: Norton & Co.
Lieberman, Philip; & Crelin, Edmund S. (1971), “On the speech of neanderthal man,” Linguistic Inquiry 2(2): 203-222.
Liégeois, Frédérique; Badelweg, Trosten; Connelly, Alan; Gadian, David G.; Mishkin, Mortimer; & Vargha-Khadem; Faraneh (2003), “Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation,” Nature Neuroscience 6(11): 1230-1237.
Lierberman, Philip (2000), Human Language and Our Reptilian Brain. The Subcortical Bases of Speech, Syntax, and Thought. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.
Lorenzo, Guillermo (2004a), “Lingüística evolutiva: hacia un enfoque modular e internista,” Ludus Vitalis. Revista de Filosofía de las Ciencias de la Vida XII(22): 153-171.
Lorenzo, Guillermo (2004b), “Sobre continuidad, especificidad, minimalismo y símbolos. Algo más sobre el enfoque modular e internista,” Ludus Vitalis. Revista de Filosofía de las Ciencias de la Vida XII(22): 203-212.
Lorenzo, Guillermo (2006), “Some minimalist thoughts on the emergence of recursiveness,” in Roselló & Martín (eds.), The Bio-Linguistic Turn. Barcelona: UB.
Lorenzo, Guillermo; & Longa, Víctor M. (2003), Homo Loquens. Biología y evolución del lenguaje. Lugo: Tris Tram.
Marcus, Gary F. (2004), The Birth of the Mind. How a Tiny Number of Genes Creates the Complexities of Human Thought. New York: Basic Books.
Marcus, Gary F.; & Fisher, Simon E. (2003), “FOXP2 in focus: what can genes tell us about speech and language,” Trends in Cognitive Science 7(6): 257-262.
McLarnon, Ann (1993), “The vertebral canal,” in A. Walker & R. Leakey (eds.) The Neriokotone Homo Erectus Skeleton. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press, pp. 359-390.
Mega, M.S.; & Alexander, M.F. (1994), “Subcortical aphasias: the core profile of capsulostriatal infarction,” Neurology 44: 1824-1829.
Mellars, Paul (1998), “Neanderthals, modern humans and the archaeological evidence for language,” in Nina C. Jablonski and Leslie C. Aiello (eds.) The Origin and Diversification of Language. San Francisco: Memoirs of the California Academy of Sciences 24: 89-115.
Mellars, Paul (2002), “Arqueología y los orígenes de los humanos modernos: perspectiva europea y africana,” in T.J. Crow (ed.) La especiación del Homo sapiens Moderno. Madrid: Triacastela, 2005, pp. 45-62.
Naeser, M.A.; Alexander, M.P.; Helms-Estabrooks, N.; Levine, H.L.; Laughlin, S.A; & Geschwind, N. (1982), “Afasia with predominantly subcortical lesion sites: description of three capsular/putaminal aphasia syndromes,” Archives of Neurology 39: 2-14.
Oppenheimer, Stephen (2003), Los senderos del Edén. Orígenes y evolución de la especie humana. Barcelona: Crítica, 2004.
Parker, Sue Taylor (1985), “A social–technological model for the evolution of language,” Current Anthropology 26: 617-626.
Perner, Josef (1991), Entender la mente representacional. Barcelona: Paidós, 1994.
Pfeiffer, Gary B. (1982), The Creative Explosion: An Inquiry into the Origins of Art and Religion. New York: Harper & Row.
Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo; & Uriagereka, Juan (2005), “The Evolution of the Narrow Faculty of Language: The Skeptical View and a Reasonable Conjecture,” Lingue e Linguaggio IV.1; 27-79.
Rizzolatti, Giacomo; Fogessi, Leonardo; & Gallese, Vittorio (2002), “Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Ventral Premotor Cortex,” Cognitive Neuroscience 12; 149-154.
Serre, D.; Langaney, A.; Chech, M.; Teschler-Nichola, M.; Paunovic, M.; Mennecier, P.; Hofreiter, M.; Possnert, G.; & Pääbo, S. (2004), “No Evidence of Neandertal mtDNA contribution to Earley Modern Humans,” PLOS Biology 2(3); 313-317.
Sperber, Dan (2000), “Metarepresentations in an Evolutionary Perspective,” in Dan Sperber (ed.) Metarepresentations. A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University, Press; 117-137.
Stringer, Christopher; & Gamble, Clive (1993), En busca de los neandertales. La solución al rompecabezas de los orígenes humanos. Barcelona: Crítica, 1996.
Takahashi, K.; Liu, F.C.; Hirokawa, K.; Takahashi, H. (2003), “Expression of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language, in the developing and adult striatum,” Journal of Neurosciences Research 73(1): 61-72.
Tatterstall, Ian (2002),, “Los sucesos saltacionistas en la evolución humana,” en T.J. Crow (ed.), La especiación del Homo Sapiens Moderno. Madrid: Triacastela, 2005, pp. 63-74.
Tobias, Phillip V. (1983), “Recent advances in the evolution of the hominids with special reference to brain and speech,” Pontificial Academy of Sciences. Scripta Varia 50: 840-951.
Tobias, Phillip V. (1987), “The brain of Homo habilis: a new level of organization in cerebral evolution,” Journal of Human Evolution 16: 741-761.
Tobias, Phillip V. (2003), “Orígenes evolutivos de la lengua hablada,” en Raúl Gutiérrez Lombardo, Jorge Martínez Contreras y José Luis Vera Cortés (eds.) Naturaleza y Diversidad Humana. Estudios Evolucionistas. México D.F.: Centro de Estudios Filosóficos, Políticos y Sociales Vicente Lombardo Toledano, pp. 285-307.
Ullman, Michael T. (2001), “A neurocognitive perspective on language: the declarative/procedural model,” Nature Reviews (Neurosciences) 2: 7-17.
Wall, Jeffrey D.; & Przeworski, Molly (2000), “When did human population size start increasing?” Genetics 155: 1865-1874.
Wilkins, Wendy K.; & Wakefield, Jennie (1995), “Brain evolution and neurolinguistic preconditions,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 161-226.
Wilson, A.C.; & Cann, R.L. (1992), “The recent African genesis of humans,” Scientific American 266: 22-27.
Wynn, Thomas; & McGrew, William C. (1989), “An ape’s view of the Oldowan,” Man 24: 383-398.
Wynn, Thomas; & Coolidge, Frederick L. (2004), “The expert Neandertal mind,” Journal of Human Evolution 46: 467-487.
Enlaces refback
- No hay ningún enlace refback.
Revista semestral editada por el Centro de Estudios Filosóficos, Políticos
y Sociales Vicente Lombardo Toledano de la Secretaría de Educación Pública,
la Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana-Iztapalapa y Edicions UIB de la Universitat de les Illes Balears.
Lombardo Toledano 51, Col. Ex-Hda. Guadalupe Chimalistac,
Del. Alvaro Obregón, C.P. 01050, México, D.F.
Tels. (5255) 5661-4679 y 5661-4987
Fax: (5255) 5661-1787