La evolución de la memoria de trabajo
Resumen
The evolution of working memory
Working memory (WM) is fundamental to many aspects of human life, including learning, speech and text comprehension, prospection and future planning, and explicit “system 2" forms of reasoning, as well as overlapping heavily with fluid general intelligence. WM has been intensively studied for many decades, and there is a growing consensus about its nature, its components, and its signature limits. Remarkably, given its central importance in human life, there has been very little comparative investigation of WM abilities across species. Consequently, much remains unknown about the evolution of this important human capacity. Some questions can be tentatively answered from the existing comparative literature. Even studies that were not intended to do so can nonetheless shed light on the WM capacities of nonhuman animals. However, many questions remain.
Key words: Attention, default network, episodic memory, mental rehearsal, primate.
Texto completo:
PDFReferencias
Allen, T., Fortin, N. (2013), “The evolution of episodic memory,” Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 10.1073/pnas.1301199110.
Baars, B.J. (2002), “The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence,” Trends Cogn Sci 6(1): 47–52.
Babb, S., Crystal, J. (2005), “Discrimination of what, when, and where: Implications for episodic-like memory in rats,” Learn Motiv 36: 177–189.
Baluch, F., Itti, L. (2011), “Mechanisms of top-down attention,” Trends Neurosci 34(4): 210–224.
Barth, J., Call, J. (2006), “Tracking the displacement of objects: A series of tasks with great apes and young children,” J Exp Psychol 32: 239–252.
Baird, B., et al. (2012), “Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation,” Psychol Sci 23(10): 1117–1122.
Bräuer, J., Call, J. (2011), “The magic cup: Great apes and domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) individuate objects according to their properties,” J Comp Psychol 125(3): 353–361.
Botvinick, M.M., et al. (2009), “An analysis of immediate serial recall performance in a macaque,” Anim Cogn 12(5): 671–678.
Buckner, R.L., (2010), “The role of the hippocampus in prediction and imagination,” Annu Rev Psychol 61: 27–48, C1–C8.
Buckner, R.L., Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Schacter, D.L. (2008), “The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease,” Ann N Y Acad Sci 1124: 1–38.
Chooi, W.-T., Thompson, L. (2012), “Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults,” Intelligence 40: 531–542.
Clayton, N.S., Yu, K.S., Dickinson, A. (2003), “Interacting Cache memories: Evidence for flexible memory use by Western Scrub-Jays (Aphelocoma californica),” J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 29(1): 14–22.
Correia, S.P., Dickinson, A., Clayton, N.S. (2007), “Western scrub-jays anticipate future needs independently of their current motivational state,” Curr Biol 17(10): 856–861.
Cowan, N. (2001), “The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity,” Behav Brain Sci 24(1): 87–114.
Cowan, N., et al. (2005), “On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes,” Cognit Psychol 51(1): 42–100.
Cowan, N., AuBuchon, A.M., Gilchrist, A.L., Ricker, T.J., Saults. J.S. (2011), “Age differences in visual working memory capacity: Not based on encoding limitations,” Dev Sci 14(5): 1066–1074.
Damasio, Anton (1994), Descartes’ Error. New York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons.
Daselaar, S.M., Porat, Y., Huijbers, W., Pennartz, C.M. (2010), “Modality-specific and modality-independent components of the human imagery system,” Neuroimage 52(2): 677–685.
Davidson, T.J., Kloosterman, F., Wilson, M.A. (2009), “Hippocampal replay of extended experience,” Neuron 63(4): 497–507.
Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., Prablanc, C. (1989), “The timing of mentally represented actions,” Behav Brain Res 34(1–2): 35–42.
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., Sergent, C. (2006), “Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy,” Trends Cogn Sci 10(5): 204–211.
Dudchenko, P.A. (2004), “An overview of the tasks used to test working memory in rodents,” Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28(7): 699–709.
Eichenbaum, H. (2013), “Memory on time,” Trends Cogn Sci 17(2): 81–88.
Engle, R. (2010), “Role of working-memory capacity in cognitive control,” Curr Anthropol 51: S17–S26.
Ericsson, K.A., Kintsch, W. (1995), “Long-term working memory,” Psychol Rev 102(2): 211–245.
Ergorul, C., Eichenbaum, H. (2004), “The hippocampus and memory for “what,” “where,” and “when”,” Learn Mem 11(4): 397–405.
Evans, J. (2010), Thinking Twice. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press.
Evans, J., Frankish, K. (eds.) (2009), In Two Minds: Dual Processes and Beyond. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press.
Fagot, J., De Lillo, C. (2011), “A comparative study of working memory: Immediate serial spatial recall in baboons (Papio papio) and humans,” Neuropsychologia 49(14): 3870–3880.
Feigenson, L., Carey, S .(2005), “On the limits of infants’ quantification of small object arrays,” Cognition 97(3): 295–313.
Ganis, G., Keenan, J.P., Kosslyn, S.M., Pascual-Leone, A. (2000), “Transcranial magnetic stimulation of primary motor cortex affects mental rotation,” Cereb Cortex 10(2): 175–180.
Gazzaley, A., Cooney, J.W., McEvoy, K., Knight, R.T., D’Esposito, M. (2005), “Top-down enhancement and suppression of the magnitude and speed of neural activity,” J Cogn Neurosci 17(3): 507–517.
Gilbert, D.T., Wilson, T.D. (2007), “Prospection: Experiencing the future,” Science 317(5843): 1351–1354.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1995), “Cellular basis of working memory,” Neuron 14(3): 477–485. 23.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S., Funahashi, S., Bruce, C.J. (1990), “Neocortical memory circuits,” Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 55: 1025–1038.
Hauser, M.D., Carey, S., Hauser, L.B. (2000), “Spontaneous number representation in semi-free-ranging rhesus monkeys,” Proc Biol Sci 267(1445): 829–833.
Hanus, D., Mendes, N., Tennie, C., Call, J. (2011), “Comparing the performance of apes and human children in the floating peanut task,” PLoS ONE 6: e19555.
Inoue, S., Matsuzawa, T. (2007), “Working memory of numerals in chimpanzees,” Curr Biol 17(23): R1004–R1005.
Jaeggi, S.M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., Perrig, W.J. (2008), “Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory,” Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105(19): 6829–6833.
Jaeggi, S.M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., Shah, P (2011), “Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training,” Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108(25): 10081–10086.
Jeannerod, M. (2006), Motor Cognition. Oxford: Oxoford Univ Press.
Jonides, J., et al. (2008), “The mind and brain of short-term memory,” Annu Rev Psychol 59: 193–224.
Kane, M.J., Hambrick, D.Z., Conway, AR (2005), “Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005),” Psychol Bull 131(1): 66–71.
Kahneman, D. (2011), Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Strauss and Giroux.
Knudsen, E.I. (2007), “Fundamental components of attention,” Annu Rev Neurosci 30: 57–78.
Kolata, S., et al. (2005), “Variations in working memory capacity predict individual differences in general learning abilities among genetically diverse mice,” Neurobiol Learn Mem 84(3): 241–246.
Kolata, S., Light, K., Grossman, H.C., Hale, G., Matzel, L.D. (2007), “Selective attention is a primary determinant of the relationship between working memory and general learning ability in outbred mice,” Learn Mem 14(1): 22–28.
Kosslyn, S. (1994), Image and Brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Light, K.R., et al. (2010), “Working memory training promotes general cognitive abilities in genetically heterogeneous mice,” Curr Biol 20(8): 777–782.
Lu, H., et al. (2012), “Rat brains also have a default mode network,” Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109(10): 3979–3984.
Luck, S.J., Chelazzi, L., Hillyard, S.A., Desimone, R. (1997), “Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex,” J Neurophysiol 77(1): 24–42.
Mason, M.F., et al. (2007), “Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought,” Science 315(5810): 393–395.
Mauck, B., Dehnhardt, G. (1997), “Mental rotation in a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus)” J Exp Biol 200(Pt 9): 1309–1316.
Matzel, L.D., et al. (2003), “Individual differences in the expression of a “general” learning ability in mice,” J Neurosci 23(16): 6423–6433.
Mendes, N., Rakoczy, H., Call, J. (2008)” “Ape metaphysics: Object individuation without language,” Cognition 106(2): 730–749.
Miller, G.A. (1956), “The magical number seven plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information,” Psychol Rev 63(2): 81–97.
Milner, D., Goodale, M. (1995), The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press.
Mulcahy, N.J., Call, J. (2006), “Apes save tools for future use,” Science 312(5776): 1038–1040.
Osvath, M. (2009), “Spontaneous planning for future stone throwing by a male chimpanzee,” Curr Biol 19(5): R190–R191.
Osvath, M., Karvonen, E. (2012), “Spontaneous innovation for future deception in a male chimpanzee,” PLoS ONE 7(5): e36782.
Osvath, M., Osvath, H. (2008), “Chimpanzee and orangutan forethought: Selfcontrol and pre-experience in the face of future tool use,” Anim Cogn 11: 661–674.
Postle, B.R. (2006), “Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain,” Neuroscience 139(1): 23–38.
Picciuto, E., Carruthers, P. (2013), “The origins of creativity,” in E. Paul E y S. Kaufman (eds.), The Philosophy of Creativity. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press.
Read, D.W. (2008), “Working memory: A cognitive limit to non-human primate recursive thinking prior to hominid evolution?,” Evol Psychol 6: 676–714.
Rilling, J.K., et al. (2007), “A comparison of resting-state brain activity in humans and chimpanzees,” Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104(43): 17146–17151.
Sanz, C., Morgan, D.., Gulick, S. (2004), “New insights into chimpanzees, tools, and termites from the Congo Basin,” Am Nat 164(5): 567–581.
Schacter, D.L., Addis, D.R., Buckner, R.L. (2007), “Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain,” Nat Rev Neurosci 8(9): 657–661.
Shettleworth, S. (2010), Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press.
Sporns, O. (2011), Networks of the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Spreng, R.N., Mar, R.A., Kim, A.S. (2009), “The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis,” J Cogn Neurosci 21(3): 489–510.
Squire, L.R. (1992), “Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans,” Psychol Rev 99(2): 195–231.
Suddendorf, T., Corballis, M.C. (2007), “The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans?,” Behav Brain Sci 30(3): 299–313.
Stanovich, K. (2009), What Intelligence Tests Miss. New Haven: Yale Univ Press.
Taylor, A.H., Elliffe, D., Hunt, G.R., Gray, RD (2010), “Complex cognition and behavioural innovation in New Caledonian crows,” Proc Biol Sci 277(1694): 2637–2643.
Uller, C., Lewis. J. (2009), “Horses (Equus caballus) select the greater of two quantities in small numerical contrasts,” Anim Cogn 12(5): 733–738.
Unsworth, N., Engle, R.W. (2007), “The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory,” Psychol Rev 114(1): 104–132.
Vauclair, J., Fagot, J., Hopkins, W. (1993), “Rotation of mental images in baboons when the visual input is directed to the left hemisphere,” Psych Sci 4(2): 99–103.
Vincent, J.L., et al. (2007), “Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain,” Nature 447(7140): 83–86.
Wolpert, D.M., Ghahramani, Z (2000), “Computational principles of movement neuroscience,” Nat Neurosci 3(Suppl): 1212–1217.
Enlaces refback
- No hay ningún enlace refback.
Revista semestral editada por el Centro de Estudios Filosóficos, Políticos
y Sociales Vicente Lombardo Toledano de la Secretaría de Educación Pública,
la Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana-Iztapalapa y Edicions UIB de la Universitat de les Illes Balears.
Lombardo Toledano 51, Col. Ex-Hda. Guadalupe Chimalistac,
Del. Alvaro Obregón, C.P. 01050, México, D.F.
Tels. (5255) 5661-4679 y 5661-4987
Fax: (5255) 5661-1787