Mechanisms of cognitive evolution of genus Homo
Resumen
The objective of this work is to analyze the biological, social and environmental mechanisms that made possible the cognitive evolution of our lineage. To this end, an interdisciplinary study is proposed along with the most influential sciences on human behavior (evolutionary biology, neurology, psychology, neurolinguistics, social anthropology, etc.). Our purpose is not to delve into any particular topic within these sciences, but to coordinate their most recent data to establish a psychobiological and social model (functional structuralism) on our neurological and cognitive evolution, which will allow us to analyze the transformations—according to the archaeological record—from the beginning of the genus Homo to the present day. The main propulsor of these changes would be causal cognition as a neurophysiological mechanism that produce the evolutionary development of human cognition (social, emotional and technological). This process, based on the exaptative character of our brain, presents a clear co-evolutionary procedure that makes possible the emergence of human cognitive capacities.
KEY WORDS. Cognitive evolution, interdisciplinary method, causal cognition
Texto completo:
PDFReferencias
Allman, J. (1990), “Evolution of neocortex”, in Cerebral Cortex (eds. A. Peters, E.G. Jones), Vol. 8. New York: Academic Press.
Álvarez Munárriz, L. (2005), “La conciencia humana”, in Munárriz, Á. Domínguez, C., (eds.). La conciencia humana: perspectiva cultural. Barcelona: Anthropos.
Ardila, A., Ostrosky-Solís, F. (2008), “Desarrollo histórico de las funciones ejecutivas”, Revista Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría y Neurociencias 8 (1): 1-21.
Baddeley, A. D. (2000), “The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory”, Trends Cogn. Sci. 4: 417-23.
Bateson, P. (2004). “The active role of behavior in evolution”. Biology and Philosophy 19 (2): 283-298.
Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A., Stocks, E. L. (2015), “The Empathy–Altruism Hypothesis”, in Schroeder, D. A., Graziano, William. G. (eds.). The Oxford Handbook of Prosocial Behavior. NY: Oxford University Press.
Belinchón, M., Igoa, J. M., Riviére, A. (1992), Psicología del lenguaje. Investigación y teoría. Madrid: Trotta.
Bercury, K. K., Macklin, W. B. (2015), “Dynamics and mechanisms of CNS myelination”. Developmental Cell 32 (4): 447-58.
Bickerton, D. (2009), Adam´s Tongue: How Humans Made Language, How Language Made Humans. New York: Hill and Wang.
Bruner, J. (1984), Acción, pensamiento y lenguaje. Madrid: Alianza.
Bruner, E., Manzi, G., Arsuaga, J. L. (2003), “Encephalization and allometric trajectories in the genus Homo: Evidence from Neandertal and modern lineages”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100: 15335-40.
Bruner, E., Lozano, M. (2014), “Extended mind and visuo-spatial integration: three hands for the Neandertal lineage”, Journal of Anthropological Sciences 92: 273-280. Retrieved from http://www.isita-org.com/jass/Contents/2014vol92/ ForumBruner/25020020.pdf
Cela Conde, C. J. (2002). “La filogénesis de los homínidos”. Diálogo filosófico 53: 228-258.
Colagè, I., d’Erricoc, F. (2018), “Culture: The driving force of human cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1–19”. In Bender, A., Beller, S., Jordan, F. (Topic Editors). The Cultural Evolution of Cognition. For a full listing of topic papers.
Retrieved from http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/10.1111/(ISSN)17568765/earlyview
Damasio, A. (2010), Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain. New York. Pantheon Books.
Dennett, D. C. (1983), “Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The ‘Panglossian paradigm’ defended”. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6 (3): 343-90.
Dennis, M. Y., Nuttle, X., Sudmant, P. H., Antonacci, F., Graves, T. A., Nefedov, M., Eichler, E. E. (2012), “Evolution of human-specific neural SRGAP2 genes by incomplete segmental duplication”. Cell 149 (4): 912–922.
Edelman, G. M., Tononi, G. (2000), A Universe of Consciousness: How Matter Becomes Imagination. New York: Basic Books.
Finlay B. L., Darlington R. B., Nicastro N. (2001), “Developmental structure in brain evolution”, Behav. Brain Sci. 24: 283-308.
Florio, M., Albert, M., Taverna, E., Namba, T., Brandl, H., Lewitus, E., Haffner, Ch., Sykes, A., Kuan Wong, F., Peters, J., Guhr, E., Klemroth, S., Prüfer, K., Kelso, J., Naumann, R., Nüsslein, I., Dahl, A., Robert Lachmann, Pääbo, S., Huttner W. B. (2015), “Human-specific gene ARHGAP11B promotes basal progenitor amplification and neocortex expansion”. Science 347 (6229): 1465-1470.
FitzPatrick, W. J. (2017), “Human altruism, evolution and moral philosophy”, Royal Society Open Science. Retrieved from http://rsos.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/royopensci/4/8/170441.full.pdf
Fuster, J. M. (2002), “Frontal lobe and cognitive development”. Journal of Neurocytology 31: 373-385.
Gallese, V., Rochat, M., Cossu, G., Sinigaglia, C. (2009), “Motor cognition and its role in the phylogeny and ontogeny of action understanding”, Dev. Psychol. 45 (1): 103-113.
Gärdenfors, P., Lombard, M. (2018), “Causal cognition, force dynamics and early hunting technologies”. Frontiers in Psychology 9, 87.
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1998), “The split brain revisited”. Scientific American 279 (1): 35-39.
Gelman, S. (1995), “Living kinds not only by movement”, in Sperber, D. and Premack, D. (eds.), Causal Cognition. New York: Sperber
Geschwind, N. (1965), “Disconnection syndromes in animal and man”. Brain: a Journal of Neurology 88 (2): 237-94.
Grimshaw, G. M., Adelstein, A.; Bryden, M. P., MacKinnon, G. E. (1998), “Firstlanguage acquisition in adolescence: evidence for a critical period for verbal language development”, Brain and Language 63 (2): 237-255.
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1998), “The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive”, in Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., Weiskrantz, L. (eds.). The Frontal Cortex: Executive and Cognitive Functions. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gómez-Pérez, E., Ostrosky-Solís, F. (2006), “Attention and memory evaluation across the life span: heterogeneous effects of age and education”, Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 28: 477-494.
Gómez-Robles, A., Hopkins, W. D., Schapiro, S. J., Sherwood, C. C. (2015), “Relaxed genetic control of cortical organization in human brains compared with chimpanzees”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112 (51): 1479914804.
Gould, S. J. (1991), “Exaptación: A crucial tool for evolutionary psychology”, Journal of Social Issues 47: 43-65.
Haidle, M. N. (2014),” Building a bridge-an archeologist’s perspective on the evolution of causal cognition”. Front. Psychol. 17 (5): 1472.
Humphrey, N. (1999), A History of the Mind: Evolution and the Birth of Consciousness. New York: Copernicus Books.
Happaney, K., Zelazo, P. D., Stuss, D. T. (2004), “Development of orbitofrontal function: current themes and future directions”. Brain and Cognition 55: 1-10.
Kandel, E. E., Schwartz, J. H., Jessell, T. M. (1995), Essentials of Neural Science and Behavior. Norwalk, CT: Appleton and Lange.
Kane, M. J., Engle R. W. (2002), “The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individualdifferences perspective”, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9 (4): 637-671.
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1964), El pensamiento salvaje. México, D.F.: Fondo de Cultura Económica.
Lombard, M., Gärdenfors, P. (2017), “Tracking the evolution of causal cognition in humans”, Journal of Anthropological Sciences 95: 1-16.
Lotem, A., Halpern, J. Y., Edelman, S., Kolodny, O. (2017), “The evolution of cognitive mechanisms in response to cultural innovations”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114 (30): 7915–7922.
Luria, A. R. (1966), Higher Cortical Function in Man. New York: Basic Books.
Luria, A., Yudovich, F. (1972), Speech and the Development of Mental Processes in the Child. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin Books.
Malafouris, L., Gosden, C., Overmann, K. A. (eds.) (2014), “Creativity, cognition and material culture”, Pragmatics and Cognition 22 (1): 1-4.
Martín-Loeches, M.; Casado, P., Sel, A. (2008), “La evolución del cerebro en el género Homo: la neurobiología que nos hace diferentes”. Rev. Neurol. 46 (12): 731-741.
Mercier, N. (2000), Words and Minds. How we Use Language to Think Together. London: Routledge.
Miller, D. J., Duka, T., Stimpson, C. D., Schapiro, S. J., Baze. W. B., McArthur, M. J., Herwood, C. C. (2012), “Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109 (41): 16480–16485.
Mora, F. (2001), El reloj de la sabiduría. Tiempos y espacios en el cerebro humano. Madrid: Alianza.
Moya-Albiol, L., Herrero, N., Bernal, M. C. (2010), “Bases neuronales de la empatía”, Rev. Neurol. 50 (2): 89-100.
Penfield, W., Rasmussen, T. (1957), Cerebral Cortex of Man, 4th ed. New York. Macmillan.
Petanjek, Z.; Judaš, M., Šimić, G.; Rašin, M. R., Uylings, H. B. M.; Rakic, P., Kostović, I. (2011), “Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex”. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108 (32): 13281–13286.
Piaget, J., Mackenzie, W. J. M., Lazarsfeld, P. F. (1973), Tendencias de la investigación en ciencias sociales. Madrid: Alianza.
Preston, S. D., de Waals, F. B. (2002), “Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 1–72.
Rakic, P. (1995), “Evolution of neocortical parcellation: the perspective from experimental neuroembryology”, in Changeux, J. P., Chavaillon J. (eds.). Origins of the Human Brain. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 85-100.
Rakic P., Kornack D. R. (2001), “Neocortical expansion and elaboration during primate evolution: a view from neuroembryology”, in Falk D., Gibson K.R. (eds.). Evolutionary Anatomy of the Primate Cerebral Cortex. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 30-56.
Renfrew, C. (2008), “Neuroscience, evolution and the sapient paradox: the factuality of value and of the sacred”. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 363: 2041-2047.
Renfrew, C., Frith, C., Malafouris, L. eds. (2009), Introduction. The Sapient Mind: Archaeology Meets Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rilling, J. K., Glasser, M. F., Preuss, T. M., Ma, X., Zhao, T., Hu, X., Behrens, T. E. (2008), “The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI”. Nature Neuroscience 11 (4): 426-428.
Rilling, J. K., Insel, T. R. (1999), “The primate neocórtex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging”. Journal of Human Evolution 37: 191-223. Rivera, A. (2015), “Arqueología de las emociones”, Vínculos de Historia 4: 41-61.
Rivera, A., Rivera, S. (2009), “Origen del lenguaje: Un enfoque multidisciplinar”, Ludus Vitalis 17 (31): 103-141.
Rivera, A., Menéndez, M. (2011), “Las conductas simbólicas en el paleolítico. Un intento de comprensión y análisis desde el estructuralismo functional”, Espacio, Tiempo y Forma. Serie I Prehistoria y Arqueología. Nueva época, 4.
Rivera, A., Rivera, S. (2017), “Estudio transdisciplinario de la autoconciencia”. Ludus Vitalis 25 (48): 155-180.
Sánchez-Cubillo, I., Tirapu Ustárroz, J., Adrover-Roig, D. (2012), “Neuropsicología de la cognición social y la autoconciencia”, in Neuropsicología del córtex prefrontal y funciones ejecutivas. J. Tirapu Ustárroz, M. Ríos-Lago, A. García Molina, A. Ardila, (eds.). Barcelona. Viguera, pp. 353-390.
Searle, J. R. (1997), The Mystery of Consciousness. New York: The New York Review of Books.
Segovia Cuéllar, A. (2017), “Reflexiones sobre la explicación evolutiva en ciencias cognitivas: el origen de la cognición social humana como estudio de caso”, Rev. Argentina de Antropología Biológica 19, 1.
Semendeferi, K., Lu, A., Schenker, N., Damasio, H. (2002), “Humans and great apes share a large frontal cortex”, Nature Neuroscience 5 (3): 272-276.
Sepulcre, J. (2014), “Functional streams and cortical integration in the human brain”, The Neuroscientist 20 (5).
Sepulcre, J., Liu, H., Talukdar, T., Martincorena, I., Yeo, B.T., Buckner, R.L. (2010), “The organization of local and distant functional connectivity in the human brain”, PLoS Comput Biol. 10, 6 (6).
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. (2013), “Afiliación, empatía y los orígenes de la teoría de la mente”, Ludus Vitalis 25 (40): 29-51.
Skoyles, J. R. (1999), “Neural plasticity and exaptation”, American Phychologist 54: 438-39.
Sporns, O. (2011), Networks of the Brain. London, Cambridge. Massachusetts: The MIT Press
Stahl, R., Walcher, T., De Juan Romero, C., Alexander Pilz, G., Cappello, S., Irmller, M., Sanz-Aquela, J. M., Beckers, J., Blum, R., Borrell, V., Götz, M. (2013), “Trnp1 regulates expansion and folding of the mammalian cerebral cortex by control of radial glial fate”, Cell 153 (3): 535–549.
Stuart-Fox, M., (2014), “The origins of causal cognition in early hominins”, Biology and Philosophy 30 (2): 247-266.
Susswein, N., Racine, T. P. (2008), “Sharing mental states”, in: Zlatev, J., Racine, T.P., Sinha, C. and Itkonen, E. (eds.). The Shared Mind: Perspectives on Intersubjectivity. Amstetrdam: John Benjamins Publishing.
Suzuki, I. K., Gacquer, D., Van Heurck, R., Kumar, D., Wojno, M., Bilheu, A.,Herpoel, A., Lambert, N., Cheron, J., Polleux, F., Detours, V., Vanderhaeghen, P. (2018), “Human-specific NOTCH2NL genes expand cortical neurogenesis through Delta/Notch Regulation”, Cell 173 (6): 1370–1384.
Tenenbaum J. B., Niyogi S. (2003), Learning Causal Laws. Proceedings of the Twenty-Fifth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. NY: American Psychological Association.
Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Muñoz-Céspedes, J. M. (2005), “Memoria y funciones ejecutivas”, Rev. Neurol. 41 (8): 475-484.
Tomasello, M. (1999), The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M. (2009), Why we Cooperate. London. The MIT press Cambridge, Mass.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., Moll, H. (2005). “Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition”. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 675–735.
Turner, J. H. (2000), On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of Human Affect. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1962), Thought and Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1978), Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Waldman, M. R. (ed.) (2017), The Oxford Handbook of Causal Reasoning. Oxford University Press.
Wood, J. N., Glynn, D. D., Phillips, B., Hauser, M. D. (2007), “The perception of rational, goal directed action in non-human primates”, Science 317, 1402.
Wynn, T., Coolidge, F. (2011), “The implications of the working memory model for the evolution of modern cognition”, International Journal of Evolutionary Biology 1-12. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3118292/ pdf/IJEB2011-741357.pdf
Enlaces refback
- No hay ningún enlace refback.
Revista semestral editada por el Centro de Estudios Filosóficos, Políticos
y Sociales Vicente Lombardo Toledano de la Secretaría de Educación Pública,
la Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana-Iztapalapa y Edicions UIB de la Universitat de les Illes Balears.
Lombardo Toledano 51, Col. Ex-Hda. Guadalupe Chimalistac,
Del. Alvaro Obregón, C.P. 01050, México, D.F.
Tels. (5255) 5661-4679 y 5661-4987
Fax: (5255) 5661-1787